The Missing Link: Here is the correct link for the article about handgun training in an era of costly ammo. http://www.lawofficer.com/news-and-articles/articles/lom/0410/what_should_i_practice.html --- One More Look at Heller: Of conservatives' few victories this year, the most cherished came when the Supreme Court, in District of Columbia v. Heller, held for the first time that the Second Amendment protects an individual right to bear arms. Now, however, a distinguished conservative jurist argues that the court's ruling was mistaken and had the principal flaws of Roe v. Wade, the 1973 abortion ruling that conservatives execrate as judicial overreaching. Both rulings, says J. Harvie Wilkinson, suddenly recognized a judicially enforceable right grounded in "an ambiguous constitutional text." ... http://townhall.com/columnists/GeorgeWill/2008/11/23/on_a_rare_08_conservative_victory?page=full --- The Beat Goes On: Jacksonville resident Scott Albrecht spent Thursday shopping for a pistol and a shotgun at St. Nicholas Gun & Sporting Goods. Albrecht wants the weapons for "personal defense," and he said the timing of his visit to the Westside gun store - 16 days after Barack Obama won the presidential election - was no coincidence. "I'm concerned that they're going to try to take away handgun rights," Albrecht said... That sentiment is shared by Mark Tushnet, a Harvard Law School professor who wrote the 2007 book Out of Range: Why the Constitution Can't End the Battle Over Guns. He told The Associated Press he thinks gun regulation is going to be a low priority for the new administration. "Maybe the gun-show loophole will be closed, but not much else," he said. The loophole he refers to is one that exempts the private sale of firearms from the federal background check requirement. That loophole means that in many states, including Florida, firearms can be sold at gun shows by private dealers without background checks... (Legally, there is no such thing as a "private dealer;" those "in the business" of selling firearms are required to be licensed. An individual who sells a firearm he no longer wants is not a dealer.) http://www.jacksonville.com/tu-online/stories/112308/met_358910637.shtml --- With Friends Like These...: The father of a 6-year-old girl who was shot while he was cleaning guns has been jailed for investigation of manslaughter... "He had sent the 6-year-old girl up to his bedroom to get a gun out of his nightstand," said Rebecca Hover of the Snohomish County Sheriff's Office. The gun was a Colt .45 Double Eagle. Police say during an interview with investigators, Peters told them when his daughter handed him the gun, he pulled out the magazine and pulled the trigger. The gun fired, and Peters' daughter dropped to the ground. She had been shot in the head. Investigators say Peters told them that while cleaning his guns, he was drinking vodka, pouring himself multiple double drinks. He told them he was probably too drunk to drive... (This was another tragic mixture of alcohol and firearms, not to mention Rule One and Rule Two violations.) http://www.king5.com/topstories/stories/NW_111708WAB_marysville_girl_shot_LJ.1bf807f2f.html --- British Device Pinpoints Sniper Fire: The palm-sized device designed by Qinetiq, the British defence firm that was once the government research laboratories, is pinned to the uniform and uses acoustic technology to calculate the exact position of the rifle fire. Then a electronic voice passes on the "bearing and range" to the soldier allowing him to jump to safety and return fire. The machine has already been purchased by the Americans for deployment in the New Year and the British are looking at a vehicle mounted version. After roadside bombs, snipers have been the biggest cause of the 301 British fatalities in both wars, and army chiefs are convinced the device could save dozens of lives... http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/newstopics/onthefrontline/3496790/Soldiers-in-Iraq-and-Afghanistan-to-get-life-saving-anti-sniper-device.html --- This Sunday...: This Sunday, on America Armed & Free, at 1 PM Dave Kopel will address what self-defense-tool owners can expect out of Attorney General nominee Eric Holder, and at 1:30 PM, Allan Gottleib of the Second Amendment Foundation on what owners of the tools of freedom can expect out of the rest of the Obamunist administration. Liberty Watch is heard Sundays from Noon - 2 PM, MST, on AM 690 in Tucson, AM 930 in Douglas, and AM 1240 in Globe. It is stramed live at, and archived at www.libertywatchradio.com/listen. Each show stays in the archive for about 4 weeks after air. --- From John Farnam: 20 Nov 08 The next layer! The feeding-frenzy at gun retailers goes on, unabated! The only problem retailers are now having is replacing depleted inventory. Every manufacturer and distributor is deep into backorder. XCRs, FALs, M1As, PTRs, SIG556s, Kalashnikovs, and ARs are all sold out. 223 and 308 ammunition is sold out. Magazines are sold out. Glocks, XDs, and most other serious pistols are sold out. Harried distributors are making only cautious promises. Items most in demand are 223 ammunition, 30-round magazines, and any rifle that takes both! Who don't now own one of the above now will have to wait months for a new one, or seriously scrounge for a used one. And, you needn't ask for a discount! Your mmunition supply is also going to be hard to find and expensive when you do. But, what we're experiencing now is just the beginning! Current panic buying is mostly on the part of those of us who already own guns and who have been closely following gun-politics. There is an another layer, and enormous one, right behind them, who are just now waking up! These are the folks who've never owned a gun, but have been "thinking about it" most of their lives. The story of pandemic panic-buying is just now reaching their ears, as it "becomes news" and subsequently breaks through in the main-stream media. As ominous anti-gun hints continue to emanate from the emerging BHO Administration, this next layer will, at long last, awaken and begin flooding gun retailers. Over the years, they've been lied to by the media, who has assured them that "guns are everywhere" and that acquiring one or two "couldn't be easier." Are they in for an unpleasant surprise! For the foreseeable future, the only "selection" they're going to see at gun retailers has already been intensely picked-over. High-demand guns will continued to depart the store the same day the come in. And, few of these folks have any idea of the convoluted paperwork jungle they'll be required to negotiate in order to legally purchase even one gun, particularly in IL, CA, MD, RI, NJ, NY and a few other states where gun purchasing and ownership is literally smothered in copious, interminable "regulations." There is no "recession" in the gun/ammunition business! The feeding-frenzy is far from over. /John (I, on the other hand, had to figure out how to fit one more S&W revolver into my safe yesterday, after getting a call from a dealer friend who knows my affinity for such equipment - he had just taken in a mint Model 15. All of us, manufacturers, dealers, friend, err at this point if we don't get these second-layer people into RKBA organizations, such as NRA and GOA, and into more training than comes in the box with the gun. Many of you who have trained with me, particularly if it was beyond the CWP course, will recall that I often commented on how to share this stuff with others. The time is near when all of us may need to function as instructors.) 21 Nov 08 From friend and colleague, Alex Robinson, of Robinson Arms in UT: "We are swamped, but we anticipated it. So, we're only six weeks out, and delivering many orders in as little as four weeks. Our biggest problem right now is finding enough staff to answer phones and e-mail!" Robinson Arms PO Bx 16776 Salt Lake City, UT 84116 801 355 0401 _www.robarm.com_ (http://www.robarm.com) /John (I'm not in a position to second John's recommendation of the XCR, having no experience with it. I share this mostly in response to "progressive" claims that the current run on guns and ammo was engineered by the manufacturers. If that were the case, wouldn't they have ramped up their production months ago?) --- From Force Science Research Center: Stress & memory: Important new findings from FSRC research Final analysis of data gathered by the Force Science Research Center during a simulated shooting experiment has revealed important new findings about officers' perceptions and recall that could bear significantly on OIS investigations. Among other things, the testing showed that: . Officers tended to recount vastly more information about what happened when interviewed by investigators than when told to write a report; . Written reports, although much briefer, were more factual than accounts given in interviews; . The error rate was considerably lower among officers who were allowed to confer briefly before being questioned; . Whether expressed orally or in writing, what officers were able to accurately remember about the confrontation was severely limited in scope by their inescapable compulsion to focus narrowly on the threat they encountered. "This is one of the most significant studies ever done in law enforcement regarding attention, memory, and memory-mining procedures," FSRC's executive director Dr. Bill Lewinski told Force Science News. "It has important implications for officers who survive gunfights, and particularly for the investigators, review boards, prosecutors, attorneys, judges, and juries who examine and assess their actions after the smoke clears." The experiment exposed 46 experienced officer volunteers from armed units of the London Metropolitan Police to a sudden confrontation with a shotgun-wielding "hostage-taker" at a training facility in England. After resolving the encounter, each officer provided an account of what had happened, either in writing or during individual interviews with I.A. investigators. The data was gathered at the end of 2006 and since then has been meticulously analyzed by FSRC's research team, headed by Lewinski. An extensive, detailed report of their findings will appear in the January 2009 issue of the professionally peer-reviewed quarterly journal Law Enforcement Executive Forum. HIGH-STRESS SCENARIO. In teams of 2 or 3, the officers (all of constable rank and all but 3 of them male) were sent to a location in the training center that had been stage-set as a hospital reception lobby, complete with staff and "waiting patients." They were told to proceed to a "locked ward" upstairs to guard a wounded armed robbery suspect. As each team walked toward a stairway, a "brother" of the suspect who wanted to visit him became unruly at the reception desk, shouting, swearing, and physically threatening the receptionist. As intended, the officers invariably became involved in this conflict. As they attempted to defuse the brother's agitation, a door to the left of the desk flew open and a role-player portraying a hostage-taker burst into the lobby, his left arm around his hostage and his right hand clutching a double-barreled shotgun. He squeezed off 2 Simunition blanks toward the floor, disengaged from the hostage, and pointed the gun at the officers. They reacted immediately to defend themselves with their Simunition-loaded Glock 17s. MEMORY TESTING. Once the encounter was resolved, the involved officers were channeled into 1 of 4 reporting options. With instructions to recount as fully as possible "everything that happened," they were assigned to handwrite reports after conferring with their teammates about the incident for up to 20 minutes, to write without conferring, or to be interviewed after conferring or not conferring. Since the conclusion of the testing, members of the research team have compiled a tedious but critical collation at FSRC's headquarters at Minnesota State University-Mankato. They have compared typed transcriptions of the officers' written reports and interview statements to videotapes from 3 cameras that ran throughout the experiment. The cameras, positioned to record the action from different angles, documented frame by frame what actually did occur. Results of this analysis, Lewinski says, is in some aspects "astounding." SELECTIVE ATTENTION. First, apart from any issues of accurate recall, the researchers "indisputably" confirmed that officers in a violent, rapidly unfolding, seemingly life-threatening situation inevitably experience what is popularly called tunnel vision or tunnel hearing, Lewinski asserts. He prefers the term "selective attention." Either way, it translates into a sharply narrowed field of sensory concentration, in which elements of the scene that do not relate directly to an officer's survival are effectively screened out by his or her brain. "It is obvious that the constables could not see everything occurring at any instant in this encounter, but the items [they] remembered at the end of the incident are a good indicator of their focus of attention during the incident," Lewinski notes in the Forum report. The average officer in the experiment was 4 times more likely to remember "external" elements associated with the threat (the type of weapon presented, the suspect's behavior, etc.) than "internal" elements (such as an awareness of his/her own thoughts and physical behavior). The more closely the "external" elements were related to the threat being presented the more likely they were to be recalled. "This narrow focus allowed the threatened officers to concentrate on what was important to them at the time--assessing and reacting to the suspect," Lewinski explains. "This appeared to greatly facilitate their performance and effectiveness." But unfortunately, this narrow focus simultaneously caused them to "miss other items about the scene that may later turn out to be important, and impaired their ability to provide full and complete reports about the incident. Because an officer doesn't focus on a certain element in the midst of fighting to save his life doesn't mean that some investigator won't focus on it later." For example, Lewinski notes, "every constable who could see the shotgun usually reported a quite detailed and accurate description of it. They tended to be accurate about the shooter's actions, as well. "But they were less accurate about the suspect's clothing and were often quite unobservant or inaccurate about the hostage. They almost never noted the behavior of anyone other than the shooter, including the location or action of his 'brother' and even other constables. They were often unaware, for example, of the number of rounds their partners fired. "In short, they were 'attentionally blind' to anything on which they were not focused." This phenomenon is something investigators and reviewers of an OIS need to keep in mind, Lewinski stresses. "An officer who says he can't remember extensive, broad details of a shooting is not necessarily being deceptive or evasive. Memories of these fast, intense events may be quite limited because the officer's focus is uncontrollably limited during their occurrence, and human beings simply can't remember what they have not noticed or paid attention to." INFORMATION VOLUME. The officers in the experiment who wrote reports about what happened "provided the least amount of information, despite being requested to write full and complete reports about everything in the incident," Lewinski reports. "They reported on the essence, providing little extra information. Their descriptions of the incident and the subjects were the barest, with little or no elaboration on such things as behavior or clothing. Their reports on their thought processes during the incident were also extremely sparse." Whether they were allowed to confer with their teammates before writing did not appear to make a difference in the extent of descriptions they provided. "What they reported was still sparse," Lewinski says. "They seemed to concentrate only on the narrowest elements related to the legality of their actions." By contrast, officers who were interviewed "provided much more information overall"--generally 2 to 4 times more than the report-writing groups. Although, like the writers, the interviewees still focused predominately on "external," threat-related items, they were notably more voluble about their own thoughts and behavior--aspects of shootings that courts and review boards often tend to be concerned about. Again, whether interviewees conferred or did not confer before giving their statements did not seem to affect the volume of information they provided. ERROR RATES. The number of items officers in the experiment incorrectly reported on "is truly astounding and definitely needs to be investigated further," Lewinski says. The lowest rate of errors in the officers' memories occurred among those who wrote reports. "The average number of errors for each constable in this category was somewhat less than half of one error per incident per constable," Lewinski reports. Officers who were interviewed, on the other hand, had error rates that were "very high," averaging more than 5 mistaken memories apiece in their accounts of what happened. The fact that the report writers as a whole volunteered less information meant that they had "less chance of being mistaken," Lewinski concedes. But this "cannot be the only reason" for the difference, which he calls "very significant." Most of the errors occurred regarding elements on the periphery of the officers' attention--items and behaviors on the fringes of or actually beyond the officers' ability to perceive them. Lewinski says it appears that interviewers pressed for more information about these things than the officers could truly produce from memory and in an effort to answer the questions, they innocently filled in the gaps with guesses or surmises that proved to be inaccurate. "This is a well-known phenomenon in memory research," Lewinski says. [To read more on this, visit the "articles" section at www.forcescience.org or simply click here for a direct link to an important article by Dr. Bill Lewinski and Dr. Audrey Honig] Apart from this study, he observes, errors tend to increase when interviewers frequently interrupt officers with questions rather than allowing them to recount what they remembered in an unbroken narrative reconstruction before posing follow-up inquiries. CONFERRING. A finding that Lewinski describes as "astounding" was the significant positive effect that conferring among teammates had on the error rate. Team members who took a few moments to discuss what happened before they then wrote a report of their experience committed the fewest memory errors of all. This group of 14 constables provided a total of 314 correct details about what they had been narrowly focused on during the action. They recorded only 2 factual errors in all that data. "An amazing statistic," Lewinski says. The error rate among officers who wrote reports without conferring, while still extremely low, was more than 4 times higher, by comparison--plus, they remembered fewer items, on average. Among interviewees, those who conferred before being questioned "were considerably more accurate in reporting those items and behaviors that they were narrowly focused on than the constables who did not confer and were interviewed." Specifically, conferring resulted in about 25% fewer errors among the interviewees, Lewinski says. The greatest differential occurred between the test subjects who wrote reports after conferring and those who were interviewed without conferring. "The error rate for those who did not confer and were interviewed is 47 times that of those who conferred and wrote reports," Lewinski says. In the forthcoming Forum article, Lewinski says that while statistical analysis indicates this finding is reliable, he is "still suspicious" of it because of the relatively small sample size in one of the experimental groups and wants to investigate it further. It's interesting to note that with the exception of one officer, those involved in the groups that conferred said they did not learn "new" information about the incident during their discussions. Rather, it seemed, latent memories they had of the incident were "refreshed" and brought to the surface by the conferencing. "In the United States," Lewinski points out, "there is a stigma attached to officers conferring about a force situation. It tends to be equated with them inappropriately 'getting their stories straight,' concocting 'facts,' or skewing the circumstances to serve their best interest. "In England, on the other hand, officers routinely confer before writing major use-of-force reports, just as partners confer before writing reports of other criminal activities by suspects. "This experiment, where the officers had little or nothing to gain by lying, strongly suggests that conferring should be regarded as a legitimate component of eliciting the richest, most complete, and most accurate memories that an officer can bring to a shooting investigation." NEW RESEARCH AHEAD. To learn more about the focus of attention and how best to evoke accurate memories of a violent encounter, FSRC will be part of a major new study in January, involving more than 300 officers from a variety of agencies and headed by Dr. Lorraine Hope, a psychologist with Portsmouth University in England. The officers will respond to a simulated incident in progress involving firearms, and then produce a written account of what happened. Some will be allowed to confer with partners before writing, others will not. In addition to the officers immediately involved, the complex incident will be witnessed by inner-city residents bused to the location and by other officers not engaged in the action. Part of this experiment will involve testing a new "guided-response" report-writing format developed by psychologists Dr. Fiona Gabbert of the University of Abertay in Scotland and Dr. Ronald Fisher of Florida International University. As with the hostage-taking scenario, this new study will be funded by the London Metropolitan Police Federation and the London Metropolitan Police Dept. In all, experts from 5 universities will be involved. Force Science News will report on findings as soon as they become available. NOTE: After publication in Law Enforcement Executive Forum, a full report on the hostage-taking experiment by Dr. Lewinski, titled "The Attention Study: A Study on the Presence of Selective Attention in Firearms Officers," will be posted on the Force Science Web site. You can also see an earlier article by Force Science News about a secondary aspect of this research, the effect that the high-stress scenario and the recounting of it afterward had on the involved officers' heart rate. Significantly, researchers discovered that being interviewed about their experience was nearly as stressful for the officers as the confrontation itself. See FSN #61 [12/15/06], "New Findings About Simulation Training and the Stress of Post-Shooting Interviews" [Click here to read the article now in the Force Science News Archive.] Visit www.forcescience.org for more information ================ The Force Science News is provided by The Force Science Institute. Subscriptions are free and sent via e-mail. To register for your free, direct-delivery subscription, please visit www.forcesciencenews.com and click on the registration button. (c) 2008: Force Science Research Institute, www.forcescience.org. Reprints allowed by request. For reprint clearance, please e-mail: info@forcescience.org. FORCE SCIENCE is a registered trademark of The Force Science Institute. ================ -- Stephen P. Wenger, KE7QBY Firearm safety - It's a matter for education, not legislation. http://www.spw-duf.info .