(TXT) View source
       
       # 2019-12-13 - The Age of Empathy by Frans De Waal
       
       I found this book in a free pile and am glad i picked it up.  I found
       it quite informative about empathy and mind-expanding in general.
       Quite fitting that i should read this after reading Ishmael.  In a
       nutshell, the book contests the idea that we are naturally bad and
       selfish to the core.  It also provides a more nuanced view of how
       empathy works and how it is organized within an individual.  My notes
       follow below.
       
       # Chapter 1, Biology Left and Right
       
       How people organize their societies may not seem the sort of topic a
       biologist should worry about.  I should be concerned with the
       ivory-billed woodpecker [thought to be extinct], the role of primates
       in the spread of AIDS or Ebola [we're primates too, right?], the
       disappearance of tropical rain forests, or whether we evolved from
       the apes. ... Every debate about society and government makes huge
       assumptions about human nature, which are presented as if they come
       straight out of biology.  But they almost never do.
       
       There is exciting new research about the origins of altruism and
       fairness in both ourselves and other animals.  For example, if one
       gives two monkeys hugely different rewards for the same task, the one
       who gets the short end of the stick simply refuses to perform. ... By
       protesting against unfairness, their behavior supports both the claim
       that incentives matter and that there is a natural dislike of
       injustice.
       
       [The author gives an example of feeding a group of chimpanzees in a
       primate research center.]
       
       My point is that there is both ownership and sharing.  In the end,
       usually within twenty minutes, all of the chimpanzees in the group
       will have some food. ... It is a relatively peaceful scene even
       though there is also quite a bit of jostling for position.
       
       So don't believe anyone who says that since nature is based on a
       struggle for life, we need to live like this as well.  Many animals
       survive by cooperating and sharing.
       
       Too many economists and politicians model human society on the
       perpetual struggle they believe exists in nature, but which is a mere
       projection. ... Obviously, competition is part of the picture, but
       humans can't live by competition alone.
       
       We live in a society that celebrates the cerebral and looks down upon
       emotions as mushy and messy.  [Never mind that emotions are just as
       cerebral as intellect.]  Worse, emotions are hard to control, and
       isn't self-control what makes us human?  [Never mind that thoughts
       are just as hard to control as emotions.  It's a rare bird who can
       turn them off like the flip of a light switch.  Or who can abruptly
       choose to believe something completely different.]
       
       Clearly, we often make snap moral decisions that come from the "gut."
       Our emotions [or subconscious] decide, after which our reasoning
       power tries to catch up as spin doctor, concocting plausible
       justifications.  With this dent in the primacy of human logic,
       pre-Kantian approaches to morality are making a comeback.  They
       anchor morality in the so-called sentiments, a view that fits well
       with evolutionary theory, modern neuroscience, and the behavior of
       our primate relatives.
       
       ... interest in others is fundamental.  Where would human morality be
       without it?  It's the bedrock upon which everything else is
       constructed.
       
       [Long-term spouses can have emotional convergence.]  Daily sharing of
       emotions apparently leads one partner to "internalize" the other, and
       vice versa...
       
       Our bodies and minds are made for social life, and we become
       hopelessly depressed in its absence.  This is why next to death,
       solitary confinement is our worst punishment.  Bonding is so good for
       us that the most reliable way to extend one's life expectancy is to
       marry and stay married.  The flip side is the risk we run after
       losing a partner.
       
       Behaviorism is the belief that behavior is all that science can see
       and know, and therefore is all it should care about.  The mind, if
       such a thing even exists, remains a black box.  Emotions are largely
       irrelevant.
       
       John Watson was the founding father of behaviorism.  He was so
       enamored by the power of conditioning that he became allergic to
       emotion.  He was particularly skeptical of maternal love, which he
       considered a dangerous instrument.  Fussing over the children,
       mothers were ruining them by instilling weaknesses, fears, and
       inferiorities.  Society needed less warmth and more structure.
       Watson dreamed of a "baby farm" without parents so that infants could
       be raised according to scientific principles.  For example, a child
       should be touched only if it has behaved incredibly well, and not
       with a hug or kiss, but rather with a little pat on the head.
       Physical rewards that are systematically meted out would do wonders,
       Watson felt, and were far superior to the mawkish rearing style of
       the average well-meaning mom.
       
       Unfortunately, environments like the baby farm existed, and all we
       can say about them is that they were deadly!  This became clear when
       psychologists studied orphans kept in little cribs separated by white
       sheets, deprived of visual stimulation and body contact.  As
       recommended by scientists, the orphans had never been cooed at, held,
       or tickled.  They looked like zombies, with immobile faces and
       wide-open expressionless eyes.  Had Watson been right, these children
       should have been thriving, but they in fact lacked all resistance to
       disease.  At some orphanages, mortality approached 100 percent.
       
       Watson's crusade against what he called the "over-kissed child," and
       the immense respect accorded him in the 1920s public opinion, seem
       incomprehensible today...
       
       Bonding is essential for our species, and it is what makes us
       happiest.  ... The pursuit of happiness written into the U.S.
       Declaration of Independence rather refers to a state of satisfaction
       with the life one is living.  This is a measurable state, and studies
       show that beyond a certain basic income, material wealth carries
       remarkably little weight.  ... time spent with friends and family is
       what does people the most good.
       
       Instead of fixating on the peaks of civilization, we need to pay more
       attention to the foothills.  The peaks glimmer in the sun, but it is
       in the foothills that we find most of what drives us, including those
       messy emotions that make us spoil our children.
       
       Security is the first and foremost reason for social life.  This
       brings me to the second false origin myth: that human society is the
       voluntary creation of autonomous people.  The illusion here is that
       our ancestors had no need for anybody else.  They led uncommitted
       lives.  Their only problem was that they were so competitive that the
       cost of strife became unbearable.  Being intelligent animals, they
       decided to give up a few liberties in return for community life.
       
       Granted, it can be instructive to look at human relations as if they
       resulted from an agreement among equal parties.  It helps us think
       about how we treat, or ought to treat, one another.  It's good to
       realize though, that this way of framing the issue is leftover from
       pre-Darwinian days, based on a totally erroneous image of our
       species. ... We descended from a long line of group-living primates
       with a high degree of interdependence.
       
       Here we arrive at the third false origin myth, which is that our
       species has been waging war for as long as it has been around.
       Although archaeological signs of individual murder go back hundreds
       of thousands of years, we lack similar evidence for warfare from
       before the agricultural revolution.
       
       Present-day hunter-gatherers alternate long stretches of peace and
       harmony with brief interludes of violent confrontation.
       
       Chimpanzees engage in violent battles over territory.  Genetically
       speaking, however, our species is exactly equally close to another
       ape, the bonobo, which does nothing of the kind.  Bonobos can be
       unfriendly to their neighbors, but soon after a confrontation has
       begun, females often rush to the other side to have sex with both
       males and other females.  Since it is hard to have sex and wage war
       at the same time, the scene rapidly turns into a sort of picnic.  It
       ends with adults from different groups grooming each other while
       their children play.  Thus far, lethal aggression among bonobos is
       unheard of.
       
       The only certainty is that our species has a potential for warfare,
       which under certain circumstances will rear its ugly head.
       
       Because of interdependencies between groups with scarce resources,
       our ancestors probably never waged war on a grand scale until they
       settled down and began to accumulate wealth by means of agriculture.
       This made attacks on other groups more profitable.  Instead of being
       the product of an aggressive drive, it seems that war is more about
       power and profit.  This also implies, of course, that it's hardly
       inevitable.
       
       [Western origin stories are not] in keeping with the old way, which
       is one of reliance on one another, of connection, of suppressing both
       internal and external disputes, because the hold on subsistence is so
       tenuous that food and safety are top priorities.
       
       We can't return to this preindustrial way of life.  We live in
       societies of mind-boggling scale and complexity that demand quite a
       different organization than humans ever enjoyed in their state of
       nature.  Yet ... we remain essentially the same animals with the same
       psychological needs and wants.
       
       False origin myths in the West: 1) Cut-throat competition is our
       natural biological state.  2) Human society is the voluntary creation
       of autonomous people.  3) Our species has been waging war for as long
       as it has been around.
       
       # Chapter 2, The Other Darwinism
       
       Long ago, American society embraced competition as its chief
       organizing principle even though everywhere one looks--at work, in
       the street, in people's homes--one finds the same appreciation of
       family, companionship, collegiality, and civic responsibility as
       everywhere else in the world.  This tension between economic freedom
       and community values is fascinating to watch, which I do both as an
       outsider and an insider, being a European who has lived and worked in
       the United States for more than twenty-five years.  The pendulum
       swings that occur at regular intervals between the main political
       parties of this nation show that the tension is alive and well, and
       that a hands-down winner shouldn't be expected anytime soon.
       
       This bipolar state of American society isn't hard to understand.
       It's not that different from the situation in Europe... What makes
       American politics baffling is the way it draws upon biology and
       religion.
       
       Evolutionary theory is remarkably popular among those on the
       conservative end of the spectrum, but not in the way biologists would
       like it to be.  The theory figures like a secret mistress.
       Passionately embraced in its obscure persona of "Social Darwinism,"
       it is rejected as soon as the daylight shines on real Darwinism.
       
       Social Darwinism depicts life as a struggle in which those who make
       it shouldn't let themselves be dragged down by those who don't.  This
       ideology was unleashed by British political philosopher Herbert
       Spencer, who in the nineteenth century translated the laws of nature
       into business language, coining the phrase "survival of the fittest"
       (often incorrectly attributed to Darwin). ... In dense tomes that
       sold hundreds of thousands of copies, he said of the poor that "the
       whole effort of nature is to get rid of such, to clear the world of
       them, and make room for better."
       
       John D. Rockefeller even married it with religion, concluding that
       the growth of a large business "is merely the working out of a law of
       nature and a law of God."  This religious angle--still visible in the
       so-called Christian Right--forms the second great paradox.  Where as
       the book found in most American homes and every hotel room [the
       Bible] urges us on almost every page to show compassion, Social
       Darwinists scoff at such feelings, which only keep nature from
       running its course.  Poverty is dismissed as proof of laziness, and
       social justice as a weakness.  Why not simply let the poor perish?  I
       find it hard to see how Christians can embrace such harsh ideology
       without a massive case of cognitive dissonance, but many seem to do
       so.
       
       The final paradox is that the emphasis on economic freedom triggers
       both the best and worst in people.  The worst is the aforementioned
       deficit in compassion, but there is also an excellent side to the
       American character--which is a merit-based society.  Americans admire
       success stories, and will never hold honest success against anyone.
       This is truly liberating for those who are up to the challenge.
       
       And so my [personal] political philosophy sits somewhere in the
       middle of the Atlantic [between American and European values]--not
       too comfortable a place.
       
       The problem is that one can't derive the goals of society from the
       goals of nature.  Trying to do so is known as the naturalistic
       fallacy, which is the impossibility of moving from how things are to
       how things ought to be.  Thus, if animals were to kill one another on
       a large scale, this wouldn't mean we have to do so, too, any more
       than we would have an obligation to live in perfect harmony if
       animals were to do so.  All that nature can offer is information and
       inspiration, not prescription.
       
       Information is critical, though.  A view of human nature as "red in
       tooth and claw" obviously sets different boundaries to society than a
       view that includes cooperation and solidarity as part of our
       background.  Darwin himself felt uncomfortable about the "right of
       the strongest" lessons that others, such as Spencer, tried to extract
       from his theory.  That is why I'm tired, as a biologist, to hear
       evolutionary theory being trotted out as a prescription for society
       by those who aren't truly interested in the theory itself and all
       that it has to offer.
       
       [Spencer] applied the naturalistic fallacy to a T.  Why did Spencer's
       ideas fall on such receptive ears?  It seems to me that he was
       offering a way out of a moral dilemma that people were only just
       getting used to.  In earlier times, the rich didn't need any
       justification to ignore the poor.  With their blue blood, the
       nobility considered itself a different BREED.  Not that they felt
       absolutely no sense of obligation toward those underneath them, but
       they had no qualms about living in opulence, feasting on meat,
       slurping fine wine, and driving around in gilded carriages, while the
       masses were close to starving.
       
       All of this changed with the Industrial Revolution, which created a
       new upper crust, one that couldn't overlook the plight of others so
       easily.  Many of them had belonged to the lower class only a few
       generations before: They were evidently of the same blood.  So,
       shouldn't they share their wealth?  They were reluctant to do so,
       though, and were thrilled to hear that there was nothing wrong with
       ignoring those who worked for them, it was perfectly honorable to
       climb the ladder of success without looking back.  This is how nature
       works, Spencer assured them, thus removing any pangs of conscience
       the rich might feel.
       
       Since the goal of every immigrant is to build a better life, the
       inevitable outcome is a culture revolving around individual
       achievement.  No wonder Spencer's message about success as its own
       justification was well received.  
       
       Insofar as such arguments are based on what is supposedly natural,
       however, they are fundamentally flawed.  In Spencer's days, this was
       exposed by the unlikely character of a Russian prince, Petr
       Kropotkin.  Though a bearded anarchist, Kropotkin was also a
       naturalist of great distinction.  In his 1902 book, Mutual Aid, he
       argued that the struggles for existence is not so much of each
       against all, but of masses of organisms against a hostile environment.
       
       Kropotkin was inspired by a setting quite unlike the one that had
       inspired Darwin.  Darwin visited tropical regions with abundant
       wildlife, whereas Kropotkin explored Siberia.  The ideas of both men
       reflect the difference between a rich environment, resulting in the
       sort of population density and competition envisioned by Malthus, and
       an environment that is frozen and unfriendly most of the time.  ...
       Instead of animals duking it out, and the victors running off with
       the prize, he saw a communal principle at work.  In subzero cold, you
       either huddle together or die.
       
       [Both Darwin and Kropotkin] believed that cooperative groups of
       animals (or humans) would outperform less cooperative ones.  In other
       words, the ability to function in a group an build a support network
       is a crucial survival skill.
       
       In chimpanzees, both males and females actively broker community
       relations.  Males who act as arbitrator usually don't take sides and
       can be remarkably effective at keeping the peace.  In all of these
       cases, primates show community concern.  They try to ameliorate the
       state of affairs in the group as a whole.  [Thus showing that
       conflict resolution is natural and requires no speech.]
       
       Every society needs to strike a balance between selfish and social
       motives to ensure that its economy serves society rather than the
       other way around.  All too often [the primacy of money] leads to
       exploitation, injustice, and rampant dishonesty.  In the past decade,
       every advanced nation has had major business scandals, and in every
       case executives have managed to shake the foundation of their society
       precisely by following Friedman's advice [of profit first].
       
       The book of nature is like the Bible: Everyone reads into it what
       they want, from tolerance to intolerance, and from altruism to greed.
       It's good to realize, though, that if biologists never stop talking
       of competition, this doesn't mean they advocate it, and if they call
       genes selfish, this doesn't mean that genes actually are.  Genes
       can't be any more "selfish" than a river can be "angry," or sun rays
       "loving."  At most, [little chunks of DNA] are "self-promoting,"
       because successful genes help their carriers spread more copies of
       themselves.
       
       The animal kingdom is full of traits that evolved for one reason but
       are also used for others. When it comes to behavior, too, the
       original function doesn't always tell us how and why a behavior will
       be used in daily life.  Behavior enjoys motivational autonomy.
       
       My main point is that even if a trait evolved for reason X, it may
       very well be used in daily life for reasons X, Y, and Z.  Offering
       assistance to others evolved to serve self-interest, which it does if
       aimed at close relatives or group mates willing to return the favor.
       This is the way natural selection operates: It produces behavior
       that, on average and in the long run, benefits those showing it.  But
       this doesn't mean that humans or animals only help one another for
       selfish reasons.  The reasons relevant for evolution don't
       necessarily restrict the actor.
       
       This is why the selfish-gene metaphor is so tricky.  By injecting
       psychological terminology into a discussion of gene evolution, the
       two levels that biologists work so hard to keep apart are slammed
       together.  Clouding the distinction between genes and motivations has
       led to an exceptionally cynical view of human and animal behavior.
       Believe it or not, empathy is commonly presented as an illusion,
       something that not even humans truly possess.
       
       Modern psychology and neuroscience fail to back these bleak views.
       We're preprogrammed to reach out.  Empathy is an automated response
       over which we have limited control.  We can suppress it, mentally
       block it, or fail to act on it, but except for a tiny percentage of
       humans--known as psychopaths--no one is emotionally immune to
       another's situation.  The fundamental yet rarely asked question is:
       Why did natural selection design our brains so that we're in tune
       with our fellow human beings, feeling distress at their distress and
       pleasure at their pleasure?
       
       Aggression was my first topic of study, and I'm fully aware that
       there's no shortage of it in the primates.  It was only later that I
       became interested in conflict resolution and cooperation.
       
       Pure, unconditional trust and cooperation are naive and detrimental,
       whereas unconstrained greed can only lead to the sort of dog-eat-dog
       world that Skilling advocated at Enron until it collapsed under its
       own mean-spirited weight.
       
       If biology is to inform government and society, the least we should
       do is get the full picture, drop the cardboard version that is Social
       Darwinism, and look at what evolution has actually put into place.
       Ideologies come and go, but human nature is here to stay.
       
       Three paradoxes in American politics:
       
       * Accepting Social Darwinism while rejecting biological Darwinism.
       * Accepting Social Darwinism in spite of professed Christian values.
       * The emphasis on economic freedom triggers both the best and worst
         in people.
       
       # Chapter 3, Bodies Talking to Bodies
       
       [Laughter and yawning are infectious even across species.]  Yawn
       contagion reflects the power of unconscious synchrony, which is as
       deeply ingrained in us as in many other animals.
       
       This is precisely where empathy and sympathy start--not in the higher
       regions of imagination, or the ability to consciously reconstruct how
       we would feel if we were in someone else's situation.  It began much
       simpler, with the synchronization of bodies...
       
       Some of these examples are more complex than mere coordination: They
       involve assuming the perspective of someone else.  Or, as in
       Goodall's and my family's account, alerting another to the situation
       of a third.  The one thread that runs through all of these examples,
       however, is coordination.  All animals that live together face this
       task, and synchrony is the key.  It is the oldest form of adjustment
       to others.  Synchrony, in turn, builds upon the ability to map one's
       own body onto that of another, and make the other's movement one's
       own, which is exactly why someone else's laugh or yawn makes us laugh
       or yawn.  Yawn contagion thus offers a hint at how we relate to
       others.  Remarkably, children with autism are immune to the yawns of
       others, thus highlighting the social disconnect that defines their
       condition.
       
       Imitation is an anthropoid forte, as reflected in the verb "to ape."
       
       Spontaneous imitation: imitating another without gains in mind.
       
       [Experiments show that ape imitation happens via identifying with one
       another and absorbing body movements, not via knowledge (technical
       know-how).]
       
       In order to learn from others, apes need to see actual fellow apes.
       Imitation requires identification with a body of flesh and blood.
       We're beginning to realize how much human and animal cognition runs
       via the body.  Instead of our brain being like a little computer that
       orders the body around, the body-brain relation is a two-way street.
       The body produces internal sensations and communicates with other
       bodies, out of which we construct social connections and an
       appreciation of the surrounding reality.  Bodies insert themselves
       into everything we perceive or think.
       
       The field of "embodied" cognition is still very much in its infancy,
       but has profound implications for how we look at human relations.
       Body-mapping is mostly hidden and unconscious...
       
       Identification is even more striking at moments of high emotion.
       
       Not only do we mimic those with whom we identify, but mimicry in turn
       strengthens the bond.  Human mothers and children play games of
       clapping hands either against each other or together in the same
       rhythm.  These are games of synchronization.  And what do lovers do
       when they first meet?  They stroll long distances side by side, eat
       together, laugh together, dance together.  Being in sync has a
       bonding effect.  Think about dancing.  Partners complement each
       other's moves, anticipate them, or guide each other through their own
       movements.
       
       This connectedness is no secret.  We explicitly emphasize it in an
       art form that is literally universal.  Just as there are no human
       cultures without language, there are none that lack music.  Music
       engulfs us and affects our mood so that, if listened to by many
       people a once, the inevitable outcome is mood convergence.  The
       entire audience gets uplifted, melancholic, reflective, and so on.
       Music seems designed for this purpose.  [There are always outliers,
       such as deaf people or neuro-diverse people who perceive audio
       differently than others.  For these people, the mood may be quite
       different.]
       
       When Katy Payne offered us the image of a human mother resonating
       with her acrobat child, she unwittingly used the same example as the
       German psychologist responsible for the modern concept of empathy.
       We're in suspense watching a high-wire artist, said Theodor Lipps
       (1851-1914), because we vicariously enter his body and thus share his
       experience.  We're on the rope with him.  The German language
       elegantly captures this process in a single noun: Einfuehlung
       (feeling into).  Later, Lipps offered empatheia as its Greek
       equivalent, which means experiencing strong affection or passion.
       British and American psychologists embraced the later term, which
       became "empathy."
       
       Such identification, argued Lipps, cannot be reduced to any other
       capacities, such as learning, association, or reasoning.  Empathy
       offers direct access to "the foreign self."  How strange that we need
       to go back one century to learn about the nature of empathy in the
       writings of a long-forgotten psychologist.  Lipps offered a bottom-up
       account, that is, one that starts from the basics, rather than the
       top-down explanations often favored by psychologists and
       philosophers.  The latter tend to view empathy as a cognitive affair
       based on our estimation of how others might feel given how we would
       feel under similar circumstances.  But can this explain the immediacy
       of our reactions?
       
       Science is coming around to Lipp's position, but this was not the
       case yet when Swedish psychologist Ulf Dimberg began publishing on
       involuntary empathy in the early 1990s.  He ran into stiff resistance
       from proponents of the more cognitive view.  Dimberg demonstrated
       that we don't decide to be empathetic--we simply are.
       
       Lipps called empathy an "instinct," meaning that we're born with it.
       
       Gender differences usually follow a pattern of overlapping bell
       curves.  Men and women differ on average, but quite a few men are
       more empathetic than the average women, and quite a few women are
       less empathetic than the average man.  With age, the empathy levels
       of men and women seem to converge.
       
       The evolution of attachment came with something the planet had never
       seen before: a feeling brain.  The limbic system was added to the
       brain, allowing emotions, such as affection and pleasure.  This paved
       the way for family life, friendships, and other caring relationships.
       
       The central importance of social bonding is hard to deny.  We have a
       tendency to describe the human condition in lofty terms, such as a
       quest for freedom or striving for a virtuous life, but the life
       sciences hold a more mundane view: It's all about security, social
       companionships, and a full belly. ... Our nobler strivings come into
       play only once the baser ones have been fulfilled.  If attachment and
       empathy are as fundamental as proposed, we had better pay close
       attention to them in any discussion of human nature.
       
       Before going any further, I must warn that reading up on the science
       of animal empathy can be a challenge for animal lovers.  To see how
       animals react to the pain of others, investigators have often
       produced the pain themselves.  I don't necessarily approve of these
       practices, and don't apply them myself, but it would be foolish to
       ignore the discoveries they've produced.  The good news is that most
       of this research was carried out decades ago, and is unlikely to be
       repeated today.
       
       Oscar the Cat stares at us from a photograph of the prestigious New
       England Journal of Medicine along with an admiring description by a
       fellow expert.  The author relates how Oscar makes his daily rounds
       at a geriatric clinic in Providence, Rhode Island, for patients with
       Alzheimer's, Parkinson's, and other illnesses.  The two-year-old cat
       carefully sniffs and observes each patient, strolling from room to
       room.  When he decides that someone is about to die, he curls up
       beside them, purring and gently nuzzling them.  He leaves the room
       only after the patient has taken her or his last breath.
       
       Oscar's predictions have been so dependable that the hospital staff
       counts on them.  If he enters a room and leaves again, they know the
       patient's time isn't up yet.  But as soon as Oscar starts one of his
       vigils, a nurse will pick up the phone to call family members, who
       then hurry to the hospital to be present while their loved one passes
       away.  The cat has predicted the deaths of more than twenty-five
       patients with greater accuracy than any human expert.  The tribute to
       the tomcat states: "No one dies on the third floor unless Oscar pays
       a visit and stays awhile."
       
 (TXT) Oscar therapy cat @Wikipedia
       
       The sight of another person's state awakens within us hidden memories
       of similar states that we've experienced.  I don't mean our conscious
       memories, but an automatic reactivation of neural circuits.  Seeing
       someone in pain activates pain circuits to the point that we clench
       our jaws, close our eyes, and even yell "Aw!" if we see a child
       scrape its knee.  Our behavior fits another's situation, because it
       has become ours.
       
       The discovery of mirror neurons boosts this whole argument at the
       cellular level.
       
       The automaticity of empathy has become a point of debate, though.
       For the same reason that Dimberg ran into resistance showing
       unconscious facial mimicry, some scientists profoundly dislike any
       talk of automaticity, which they equate with "beyond control."  We
       can't afford automatic reactions, they say.  If we were to empathize
       with everybody in sight, we'd be in constant emotional turmoil.  I'd
       be the last to disagree, but is this really what "automaticity"
       means?  It refers to the speed and subconscious nature of a process,
       not the inability to override it.  My breathing, for instance, is
       fully automated, yet I remain in charge.  This very minute, I can
       decide to stop breathing until I see purple.  [This is a profound
       point that relates focus on the breath to a path toward
       self-realization.]
       
       The ability to control and inhibit responses is not our only weapon
       against rampant empathy.  We also regulate it at its very source by
       means of selective attention and identification.  If you don't want
       to be aroused by an image, just don't look at it.  And even though we
       identify easily with others, we don't do so automatically.  For
       example, we have a hard time identifying with people whom we see as
       different or belonging to another group. ... Identification is such a
       basic precondition for empathy that even mice shown pain contagion
       only with their cage mates.
       
       If identification with others opens the door for empathy, the absence
       of identification closes that door.
       
       Empathy can also be nipped in the bud.  People who are perfectly
       attached and sensitive in one context may act like monsters in
       another.
       
       But even if empathy is hardly inevitable, it is automatically aroused
       with those who have been "preapproved" based on similarity or
       closeness.  With them, we can't help resonating.
       
       ... the face remains the emotion highway: It offers the quickest
       connection to the other.  Our dependence on this highway may explain
       why people with immobile or paralyzed faces feel deeply alone, and
       tend to become depressive, sometimes to the point of suicide.
       
       # Chapter 4, Someone else's shoes
       
       American Psychologist Lauren Wispe offers the following definition:
       The definition of sympathy has two parts: first, a heightened
       awareness of the feelings of the other person, and, second, an urge
       to take whatever actions are necessary to alleviate the other
       person's plight.
       
       Thus, while empathy is easily aroused, sympathy is a separate process
       under quite different controls.  It is anything but automatic.
       Nevertheless, it is common in both humans and other animals.
       
       Ironically, this has been clear for a long time, but developments
       have conspired against it becoming widely known.  First of all, until
       recently empathy was not taken seriously by science.  Even with
       regard to our own species, it was considered an absurd, laughable
       topic classed with supernatural phenomena such as astrology and
       telepathy.  A trailblazing child-empathy researcher once told me
       about the uphill battle to get her message across thirty years ago.
       Everything connected with empathy was seen as ill-defined,
       bleeding-heart kind of stuff, more suitable for women's magazines
       than hard-nosed science.
       
       With regard to animals, the same resistance still exists.
       
       targeted helping: assistance geared toward another's specific
       situation or need.  [Basically the platinum rule: Do unto others as
       they would have done unto them.]  I believe that apes are masters at
       this kind of insightful help.
       
       Preconcern is an attraction toward anyone whose agony affects you.
       It doesn't require imagining yourself in another's situation, and
       indeed the capacity to do so may be wholly absent...
       
       With preconcern in place, learning and intelligence can begin to add
       layers of complexity, making the response even more discerning until
       full-blown sympathy emerges.  Sympathy implies actual concern for the
       other and an attempt to understand what happened.  The observer tries
       to figure out the reason for the other's distress, and what might be
       done about it.  Since this is the level of sympathy that we, human
       adults, are familiar with, we think of it as a single process, as
       something you either have or lack.  But in fact, it consists of many
       different layers added by evolution over millions of years.  Most
       mammals show some of these--only a few show them all.
       
       ... which is the sort of perspective taking often referred to as
       "theory of mind."  Rock seemed to have an idea (a theory) of what
       might be going on in Belle's head.
       
       I like to call it [sympathy] "cold" perspective taking, because it
       focuses entirely on how one individual perceives what another sees or
       knows.  It doesn't concern itself much with what the other wants,
       needs, or feels.  Cold perspective-taking is a great capacity to
       have, but empathy rests on a different kind, geared more toward the
       other's situation and emotions.
       
       It is this combination of emotional arousal, which makes us care, and
       a cognitive approach, which helps us appraise the situation, that
       marks empathic perspective-taking.  Only when both processes are
       combined can an organism move from preconcern to actual concern,
       including the targeted helping typical of our close relatives [other
       primates].
       
       There is in fact so much evidence for altruism in apes that i will
       pick just a handful of stories to drive my point home.
       
       Commitment to others, emotional sensitivity to their situation, and
       understanding what kind of help might be effective is such a human
       combination that we often refer to it as being humane.  I do believe
       that our species is special in the degree to which it puts itself
       into another's shoes.  We grasp how others feel and what they might
       need more fully than any other animal.  Yet our species is not the
       first or only one to help others insightfully.  Behaviorally
       speaking, the difference between a human and ape jumping into water
       to save another isn't that great.  Motivationally speaking, the
       difference can't be that great, either.
       
       Psychologists may want a rational evaluation, but children have a
       hard time extracting themselves from a confrontation with a
       salivating predator.  Only by age of seven or eight do they manage
       such distance...  Instead of staying neutral, children tend toward
       empathy.  This primal connection takes over if anyone they feel close
       to gets in trouble...  Children read "hearts" well before they read
       minds.
       
       low-cost altruism is when one isn't going much out of the way for
       someone else but still offers substantial help.  Under hardship, the
       cost of civility goes up.
       
       Perhaps it is time to abandon the idea that individuals faced with
       others in need decide whether to help, or not, by mentally tallying
       up costs and benefits.  These calculations have likely been made for
       them by natural selection.  Weight the consequences of behavior over
       evolutionary time, it has endowed primates with empathy, which
       ensures that they help others under the right circumstances.  The
       fact that empathy is most easily aroused by familiar partners
       guarantees that assistance flows chiefly toward those close to the
       actor.  Occasionally, it may be applied outside this inner circle,
       such as when apes help ducklings or humans, but generally primate
       psychology has been designed to care about the welfare of family,
       friends, and partners.
       
       Humans are empathic with partners in a cooperative setting, but
       "counterempathic" with competitors.  Treated with hostility, we show
       the opposite of empathy.  Instead of smiling when the other smiles,
       we grimace as if the other's pleasure disturbs us.  When the other
       shows signs of distress, on the other hand, we smile, as if we enjoy
       their pain.  ... So human empathy can be turned into something rather
       unattractive if the other's welfare is NOT in our interest.
       
       If helping is based on what we feel, or how we connect with the
       victim, doesn't it boil down to helping ourselves?  If we feel a
       "warm glow," a pleasurable feeling at improving the plight of others,
       doesn't this in fact make our assistance selfish?  The problem is
       that if we call this "selfish," then literally everything becomes
       selfish and the word loses its meaning.  Yes, we derive pleasure from
       helping others, but since this pleasure reaches us VIA the other and
       ONLY via the other, it is genuinely other-oriented.
       
       # Chapter 5, The elephant in the room
       
       With an anatomy so different from ours, the ease with which elephants
       arouse human sympathy poses yet another version of the correspondence
       problem.  How do we map their bodies onto ours?
       
       Advanced empathy is unthinkable without a sense of self, which is
       what mirror tests get at.  Human babies don't recognize themselves in
       a mirror right away.  A one-year-old is as confused as many animals
       about the "other" in the mirror, often smiling at, patting, even
       kissing their reflection.  They usually pass the so-called rouge test
       in front of the mirror by age two, rubbing off a dab of makeup that
       has been put on their face.  They don't know about the dab until they
       look into the mirror, so when they touch it, we can be sure they
       connect their reflection with themselves.
       
       Around the same time children pass the rouge test, they become
       sensitive to how others look at them, show embarrassment, use
       personal pronouns, and develop pretend play in which they act out
       little scenarios with toys and dolls.  These developments are linked.
       Children passing the rouge test use more "I" and "me" and show more
       pretend play than children failing this test.
       
       Since all of these abilities emerge at the same time as mirror
       self-recognition, I'll speak of the co-emergence hypothesis.
       Advanced empathy belongs to the same package.  Why should caring for
       others begin with the self?  There is an abundance of rather vague
       ideas about this issue, which I am sure neuroscience will one day
       resolve.
       
       Like apes, [the elephants] used the mirror to inspect parts of their
       bodies that they normally never see.  They opened their mouths wide
       in front of the mirror, feeling into them with their trunks.  The
       great thing, compared with dolphins, is that the elephant is an
       animal that can touch itself.  [An elephant named Happy passed the
       test.  Two more elephants failed the test.]  This is less surprising
       than it may seem because for even the most intensely tested primate,
       the chimpanzee, the proportion of individuals passing the rouge test
       is far from 100 percent, and in some studies it is less than half.
       But the funniest opening line came from a widely carried Associated
       Press piece: "If you're Happy and you know it, pat your head."
       
       ... as it turns out, all mammals with mirror self-recognition possess
       a rare type of brain cell.
       
       A decade ago, a team of neuroscientists showed that so-called Ven
       Economo neurons, or VEN cells, are limited to the hominoid (human and
       ape) brain.  VEN cells differ from regular neurons in that they are
       long and spindle-like.  They reach further and deeper into the brain,
       making them ideal to connect distant layers. ... In the dissection of
       the brains of many species, these cells were found only in humans and
       their immediate relatives, but were absent in all other primates,
       such as monkeys.  The cells are particularly large and abundant in
       our own species, and are found in a part of the brain critical for
       traits that we consider "humane."  Damage to this particular part
       results in a special kind of dementia marked by the loss of
       perspective-taking, empathy, embarrassment, humor, and
       future-orientation.  Most important, these patients also lack
       self-awareness.
       
       In other words, when humans lose their VEN cells, they lose about
       every capacity that's part of the co-emergence hypothesis.  It's
       unclear if these particular cells themselves are responsible, but it
       is thought that they underpin the required brain circuitry.
       
       ... the latest discovery by Allman's team is that VEN cells are not
       limited to humans and apes.  These neurons have made their
       independent appearance in only two other branches of the mammalian
       tree, which happen to be the cetaceans (dolphins and whales) and
       elephants.
       
       [The author studied consolation in monkeys.]  But to our surprise, we
       found nothing!  Whereas reconciliation, in which former opponents
       come together, occurs in all monkeys studied, consolation is totally
       absent.
       
       There are other indicators of a lack of empathy in monkeys, such as
       the "exasperating" stories of baboon watchers in the Okavango Delta
       of Botswana about adults ignoring the fear of youngsters facing a
       water crossing.  Standing panicky at the water's edge, young baboons
       risk getting killed by predators, yet their mothers rarely return to
       retrieve them.  They just keep traveling.  It's not that a baboon
       mother is entirely indifferent:
       
       "She appears genuinely concerned by its agitated screams.  But seems
       to fail to understand the cause of this agitation.  She behaves as if
       she assumes if she can make the water crossing, everyone can make the
       water crossing.  Other perspectives cannot be entertained."
       
       All of this suggests intact emotional contagion but an inability to
       adopt another's point of view.  This is a familiar deficit in many
       animals as well as young children.
       
       The limited sensitivity of monkeys to others seems due more to
       cognitive than emotional factors.  Monkeys do feel the distress of
       others but have no good grasp of what's going on with them.  They
       can't step back from the situation to figure out the other's needs.
       Every monkey lives in its own little bubble.
       
       Monkeys are able to use a mirror to locate food.  Many a dog can do
       the same...  It is specifically the relation with their own body,
       their own self, that they fail to grasp.
       
       On the other hand, the standard claim that monkeys see a stranger in
       the mirror is questionable.  They treated their mirror image quite
       differently from real monkeys [behind plexiglass] and did so within
       seconds.  They didn't need any time to notice the difference.
       Apparently, there are many levels of mirror understanding, and our
       [possessive] monkeys never confused their reflection with another
       monkey's. ... For me, the most telling finding of the whole study was
       that when there was a stranger on the other side, mothers held their
       infants tight, not letting them wander around.  During mirror tests,
       on the other hand, they let their kids roam freely.  Given how
       conservative mother monkeys are when it comes to danger, this
       convinced me more than anything that their reflection was no stranger
       to them.
       
       ... a recent study has shown mirror recognition in magpies.
       Follow-up research with Nathan Emery at Cambridge University led to
       the intriguing claim that "it takes a thief to know a thief."  Jays
       apparently extrapolate from their own experiences to the intentions
       of others, so that those who in the past have misappropriated the
       caches of others are especially keen on keeping the same thing from
       happening to themselves.  Perhaps this process, too, requires the
       ability to parse the self from the other.  As the ultimate bird
       thief, the magpie may have an even greater need to guess the
       intentions of others.  Curiously, their self-recognition may
       therefore relate to a life of crime.
       
       What makes information-sharing interesting is that it relies on the
       same comparison of one's own perspective with that of someone
       else--detecting something that others need to know about--which also
       underlies advanced empathy.
       
       If i point out an animal in the distance and say "zebra," and you
       disagree, saying "lion," we have a problem that, at other times, may
       get us into deep trouble.  It's a uniquely human problem, but so
       important to us that the diectic gestures and language evolution are
       closely inter-twined.
       
       # Chapter 6, Fair is fair
       
       We walk on two legs, a social and a selfish one.  We tolerate
       differences in status and income only up to a degree, and begin to
       root for the underdog as soon as this boundary is overstepped.  We
       have a deeply ingrained sense of fairness, which derives from our
       long history as egalitarians.
       
       [Unlike other apes,] Our species has a distinctly subversive streak
       that ensures that, however much we look up to those in power, we're
       always happy to bring them down a peg.
       
       Even Sigmund Freud recognized this unconscious desire, speculating
       that human history began when frustrated sons banded together to
       eliminate their imperious father, who kept all women away from them.
       The sexual connotations of Freud's origin story may serve as a
       metaphor for all our political and economic dealings, a connection
       confirmed by brain research.  Wanting to see how humans make
       financial decisions, economists found that while weighing monetary
       risks, the same areas in men's brains light up as when they're
       watching titillating sexual images.  In fact, after having seen such
       images, men throw all caution overboard and gamble more money than
       they normally would.  In the words of one neuroscientist, "The link
       between sex and greed goes back hundreds of thousands of years, to
       men's evolutionary role as provider or resource gatherer to attract
       women."
       
       Frank believes that a purely selfish outlook is, ironically, not in
       our own best interest.  It narrows our view to the point that we're
       reluctant to engage in the long-term emotional commitments that have
       served our lineage so well for millions of years.  If we truly were
       the cunning schemers that economists say we are, we'd forever be
       hunting hare, whereas our prey would be stag.
       
       It's the choice between the small rewards of individualism and the
       large rewards of collective action.  Productive partnerships require
       a history of give-and-take and proven loyalty.  Only then do we
       accomplish goals larger than ourselves.
       
       The difference is dramatic.  In 1953, eight mountaineers got into
       trouble on K2, one of the highest and most dangerous peaks in the
       world.  In -40 degrees Celsius temperatures, one member of the team
       developed a blood clot in his leg.  Even though it was
       life-threatening for the others to descend with an incapacitated
       comrade, no one considered leaving him behind.  The solidarity of
       this group has gone down in history as legendary.  Contrast this with
       the recent drama, in 2008, in which eleven mountaineers perished on
       K2 after having abandoned their common cause.  One survivor lamented
       the drive for self-preservation: "Everybody was fighting for himself,
       and I still don't understand why everybody were [sic] leaving each
       other."
       
       The first team was hunting stag, the second hare.
       
       Trust is defined as reliance on the other's truthfulness or
       cooperation, or at least the expectation that the other won't dupe
       you.  Trust is the lubricant that makes a society run smoothly.  If
       we had to test everyone all the time before doing something together,
       we'd never achieve anything.  We use past experiences to decide whom
       to trust, and sometimes rely on generalized experience with members
       of our society.
       
       The main message of this study, and many others, is that our species
       is more trusting than predicted by rational-choice theory.
       
       Confidence in others may be fine in a one-shot game with little
       money, but in the long run we need to be more careful.  The problem
       with any cooperative system is that there are those who try to get
       more out of it than they put in.  The whole system will collapse if
       we don't put a halt to freeloading, which is why humans are naturally
       cautious when they deal with others.
       
       Strange things happen if this caution is lacking.  A tiny proportion
       of humans is born with a genetic defect that makes them open and
       trusting to anyone.  These are patient with Williams syndrome, a
       condition caused by the nonexpression of a relatively small number of
       genes on the seventh chromosome.  Williams syndrome patients are
       infectiously friendly, highly gregarious, and incredibly verbose.
       
       Even though it is hard to resist these charming children, they lack
       friends.  The reason is that they trust everyone indiscriminately and
       love the whole world equally.  We withdraw from such people since
       we don't know whether we can count on them.  Will they be grateful
       for received favors, will they support us if we get into a fight,
       will they help us achieve our goals?  Probably none of the above,
       which means that they don't have anything that we're looking for in a
       friend.  They also lack the basic social skill of detecting the
       intentions of others.  They never assume wrong intentions.
       
       Williams syndrome is an unfortunate experiment of nature that shows
       that just being friendly and trusting, which is what these patients
       excel at, is not sufficient for lasting ties: We expect people to be
       discriminating.  That a small number of genes can cause such a defect
       tells us that the normal tendency to be circumspect is inborn.  Our
       species carefully chooses between trust and distrust, as do many
       other species.
       
       Large cities are obviously a different story.  Think back to 1997,
       when a Danish mother left her fourteenth-month-old girl in a stroller
       outside a Manhattan restaurant.  Her child was taken into custody and
       placed in foster care, while the mother ended up in jail.  For most
       Americans, she was either crazy or criminally negligent, but in fact
       the mother merely did what Danes are used to.  Denmark has incredibly
       low crime rates, and parents feel that what a child needs most is
       fresh air.  The mother counted on safety and good air, whereas New
       York offered neither.  The charges against her were eventually
       dropped.
       
       The faith that Danes unthinkingly place in one another is known as
       "social capital," which may well be the most precious capital there
       is.  In survey after survey, Danes have the world's highest happiness
       score.
       
       But an even simpler solution is to avoid those who are short on
       gratitude.  If one can choose between multiple partners, why not just
       go with the good ones, who can be trusted to respect past exchanges
       rather than those lousy freeloaders whom we can all do without?  We
       are like the clientele of a market where we pick and choose our
       partnerships, squeezing and smelling them the way the French do with
       cantaloupes.  We want the best ones.
       
       Have you ever noticed how often politicians lift infants above the
       crowd?  It's an odd way of handling them, not always enjoyed by the
       object of attention itself.  But what good is a display that stays
       unnoticed?
       
       One school of thought proposes that our ancestors became such great
       team players because of their dealings with strangers.  This forced
       them to develop reward and punishment schemes that worked even with
       outsiders whom they had never met and would never see again.  It is
       well known that human strangers brought together in the laboratory
       adopt strict rules of cooperation and turn against anyone who fails
       to comply.  This is known as strong reciprocity.
       
       If humans show strong dispositions towards fairness in one-shot,
       anonymous encounters, this hardly means that these dispositions
       evolved to function in one-shot, anonymous encounters any more than
       we would argue that children's strong emotional reactions to cartoons
       show that such reactions evolved in the context of cartoons.
       
       Fairness is viewed differently by the haves and have-nots.  We're all
       for fair play so long as it helps us.  There's even a biblical
       parable about this, in which the owner of a vineyard rounds up
       laborers at different times of the day.  Early in the morning, he
       goes out to find men, offering each one a denarius for their labor.
       He goes again in the middle of the day, offering the same.  At the
       "eleventh hour" he hires a few more with the same deal.  By the end
       of the day, he pays all of them, starting with the last ones hired.
       Each one gets a denarius.  Watching this, the other workers expect to
       get more since they had worked through the heat of the day.  Yet they
       get paid one denarius as well.  The owner doesn't feel he owes them
       any more than what he had promised.  The passage famously concludes
       with "So the last will be first, and the first will be last."
       
       Again, the grumbling was one-sided: It came from the early hires.
       The potential for green-eyed reactions is the chief reason why we
       strive for fairness even when we have the advantage.
       
       Privileges are always enjoyed under a cloud.  Human history is filled
       with "let them eat cake" moments that create resentment, sometimes
       boiling over into bloody revolt.  The main reason humans seek
       fairness, I believe, is to prevent such negative reactions.
       
       > If you want peace, work for justice.  --H.L. Mencken, Sage of
       > Baltimore.
       
       What confuses some is that fairness has two faces.  Income equality
       is one, but the connection between effort and reward is another.  Our
       [possessive] monkeys are sensitive to both, as are we.
       
       Richard Wilkinson, the British epidemiologist and health expert who
       first gathered these statistics, has summarized them in two words:
       "inequality kills."  He believes that the income gaps produce social
       gaps.  They tear societies apart by reducing mutual trust, increasing
       violence, and inducing anxieties that compromise the immune systems
       of both the rich and the poor.  Negative effects permeate the entire
       society...
       
       The context of an industrialized multilayered society is new but the
       emotional undercurrent of these encounters is a primate universal.
       Modern society taps into a long history of hierarchy formation in
       which those lower on the scale not only fear the higher-ups but also
       resent them.
       
       Robin Hood had it right.  Humanity's deepest wish is to spread the
       wealth.
       
       # Chapter 7, Crooked timber
       
       Asked by a religious magazine what I would change about the human
       species "if I were God," I had to think hard.  Every biologist knows
       the law of unintended consequences, a close cousin of Murphy's law.
       Any time we fiddle with an ecosystem by introducing new species, we
       create a mess.
       
       We have seen in Romanian orphanages what happens when children are
       subjected to the baby-factory ideas of behaviorist psychology.  I
       remain deeply skeptical of any "restructuring" of human nature even
       though the idea has enjoyed great appeal over the ages.
       
       Humans are bipolar apes.  We have something of the gentle, sexy
       bonobo, which we may like to emulate, but not too much; otherwise the
       world might turn into one giant hippie fest of flower power and free
       love.  Happy we might be, but productive perhaps not.  [Sounds OK to
       me!]  And our species also has something of the brutal, domineering
       chimpanzee, a side we may wish to suppress, but not completely,
       because how else would we conquer new frontiers and defend our
       borders?
       
       So strange as it may sound, I'd be reluctant to radically change the
       human condition.  But if I could change one thing, it would be to
       expand the range of fellow feeling.  The greatest problem today, with
       so many different groups rubbing shoulders on a crowded planet, is
       excessive loyalty to one's own nation, group, or religion.  Humans
       are capable of deep disdain for anyone who looks different or thinks
       another way, even between neighboring groups with almost identical
       DNA, such as the Israelis and Palestinians.
       
       Asked why he never talked about the number of civilians killed in the
       Iraq War, U.S. defense secretary Donald Rumsfeld answered: "Well, we
       don't do body counts on other people."
       
       Empathy for "other people" is the one commodity the world is lacking
       more than oil.  It would be great if we could create at least a
       modicum of it. ... If I were God, I'd work on the reach of empathy.
       
       Empathy is multilayered like a Russian doll, with at its core the
       ancient tendency to match another's emotional state (emotional
       contagion).  Around this core, evolution has built ever more
       sophisticated capacities... Concern for others (consolation).
       Perspective-Taking (targeted helping).
       
       I derive great optimism from empathy's evolutionary antiquity.  It
       makes it a robust trait that will develop in virtually every human
       being so that society can count on it and try to foster and grow it.
       It is a human universal.  In this regard, it's like our tendency to
       form social hierarchies, which we share with so many animals, and
       which we don't need to teach or explain to children: They arrange
       themselves spontaneously into pecking orders before we know it.
       
       Have you heard of an organization that appeals to empathy in order to
       fight the lack of it?  That the world needs such an organization,
       known as Amnesty International, says a lot about the dark side of our
       species.
       
       In and of itself, taking another's perspective is a neutral capacity:
       It can serve both constructive and destructive ends.  Crimes against
       humanity often rely on precisely this capacity.
       
       Torture requires an appreciation of what others think or feel.
       [Examples of torture techniques, they all rest] on our ability to
       assume their [the victim's] viewpoint and realize what will hurt or
       aggravate them the most.  Cruelty, too, rests on perspective-taking.
       
       The comparison with snakes is apt, since psychopaths seem to lack the
       Russian doll's old mammalian core.  They do possess all of its
       cognitive outer layers, allowing them to understand what others want
       and need as well as what their weaknesses are, but they couldn't care
       less about how their behavior impacts them.
       
       A lot of trouble in the world can be traced to people whose Russian
       doll is an empty shell.  Like aliens from another planet, they are
       intellectually capable of adopting another's viewpoint without any of
       the accompanying feelings.  They successfully fake empathy.
       
       To actually kill someone is, of course, quite different from watching
       a movie about it, and in this regard the data tell us something few
       would have suspected: Most men lack a killer instinct.
       
       It is a curious fact that the majority of soldiers, although well
       armed, never kill.  Killing or hurting others is something we find so
       horrendous that wars are often a collective conspiracy to miss, an
       artifice of incompetence, a game of posturing rather than an actual
       hostile confrontation.  Nowadays, this is not always realized, given
       that wars can be fought at a distance almost like a computer game,
       which eliminates most of these natural inhibitions.  But actual
       killing at close range has no glory, no pleasure, and is something
       the typical soldier tried to avoid at all cost.  Only a small
       percentage of men--perhaps 1 or 2 percent--does the vast majority of
       killing during a war.
       
       So anyone who would like to use war atrocities as an argument against
       human empathy needs to think twice.  The two aren't mutually
       exclusive, and it's important to consider how hard most men find it
       to pull the trigger.
       
       Mencius, a follower of Confucius, saw empathy as part of human
       nature, famously stating that everyone is born with a mind that
       cannot bear to see the suffering of others.  We care more about what
       we see firsthand than about what remains out of sight.  Mencius made
       us reflect on the origin of empathy, and how much it owes to bodily
       connections.  These connections also explain the trouble we have
       empathizing with outsiders.  Empathy builds on proximity, similarity,
       and familiarity, which is entirely logical given that it evolved to
       promote in-group cooperation.
       
       The firmest support for the common good comes from enlightened
       self-interest: the realization that we're all better off if we work
       together.  If we don't benefit from our contributions now, then at
       least potentially we will in the future, and if not personally, then
       at least via improved conditions around us.  Since empathy binds
       individuals together and gives each a stake in the welfare of others,
       it bridges the world of direct "what's in it for me?" benefits and
       collective benefits, which take a bit more reflection to grasp.
       Empathy has the power to open our eyes to the latter by attaching
       emotional value to them.
       
       One of the most potent weapons of the abolitionist movement were
       drawings of slave ships and their human cargo, which were
       disseminated to generate sympathy and moral outrage.  The role of
       compassion in society is therefore not just one of sacrificing time
       and money to relieve the plight of others, but also of pushing a
       political agenda that recognizes everyone's dignity.  Such an agenda
       helps not merely those who need it most, but also the larger whole.
       One can't expect high levels of trust in a society with huge income
       disparities, huge insecurities, and a disenfranchised underclass.
       And remember, trust is what citizens value most in their society.
       
       author: Waal, F. B. M. de (Frans B. M.), 1948-
 (HTM) detail: http://www.emory.edu/LIVING_LINKS/empathy/
       LOC:    BF575.E55 W3
       tags:   book,inspiration,non-fiction
       title:  The Age of Empathy
       
       # Tags
       
 (DIR) book
 (DIR) inspiration
 (DIR) non-fiction